The artificial system also revealed new information about this common illusion: the speed of reverse-phi motion is affected by how far apart the dots are, in the reverse to what would be expected. Dots ‘moving’ at a constant speed appear to move faster if spaced a short distance apart, and more slowly if spaced a longer distance apart.
“We’ve known about reverse-phi motion for a long time, but the new model generated a completely new prediction about how we experience it, which no-one has ever looked at or tested before,” said Dr Reuben Rideaux, a researcher in the University of Cambridge’s Department of Psychology and first author of the study.
Humans are reasonably good at working out the speed and direction of a moving object just by looking at it. It’s how we can catch a ball, estimate depth, or decide if it’s safe to cross the road. We do this by processing the changing patterns of light into a perception of motion – but many aspects of how this happens are still not understood.
“It’s very hard to directly measure what’s going on inside the human brain when we perceive motion - even our best medical technology can’t show us the entire system at work. With MotionNet we have complete access,” said Rideaux.
Thinking things are moving at a different speed than they really are can sometimes have catastrophic consequences. For example, people tend to underestimate how fast they are driving in foggy conditions, because dimmer scenery appears to be moving past more slowly than it really is. The researchers showed in a previous study that neurons in our brain are biased towards slow speeds, so when visibility is low they tend to guess that objects are moving more slowly than they actually are.
Revealing more about the reverse-phi illusion is just one example of the way that MotionNet is providing new insights into how we perceive motion. With confidence that the artificial system is solving visual problems in a very similar way to human brains, the researchers hope to fill in many gaps in current understanding of how this part of our brain works.
Predictions from MotionNet will need to be validated in biological experiments, but the researchers say that knowing which part of the brain to focus on will save a lot of time.
Rideaux and his study co-author Dr Andrew Welchman are part of Cambridge’s Adaptive Brain Lab, where a team of researchers is examining the brain mechanisms underlying our ability to perceive the structure of the world around us.